How Does Valuing Money Affect Your Happiness?
Two new studies find that tying your self-worth to financial success
hampers happiness and well-being—even for the well-off.
It may seem that money is a sure path to
prestige and happiness. After all, many of our most well-paid citizens are held
up as role models of success, leading seemingly perfect, enviable lives. Still,
some people embrace the opposite idea: Money can’t buy you happiness. So, which
of these is right?
In recent studies, scientists have found that
the connection between wealth and well-being is not clear-cut. While some studies
seem to tie wealth to well-being, others show
that, after a certain point, a higher income will not bring more happiness or
life satisfaction.
Now two new studies shed further light on the
relationship between wealth and happiness. Their findings suggest that money
doesn’t fulfill basic psychological needs, like belonging and competence.
That’s why making more of it will not increase your happiness, even if you
value money above other things. In fact, it may do the opposite.
What
money can and can’t do for you
In one study, University of
Buffalo researcher Lora Park and her colleagues investigated what happens when
people tie their self-worth to financial success, scoring high on the
“Financial Contingency of Self-Worth” scale, or FCWS. The researchers found
that doing so made people engage in more social comparisons, experience more
stress and anxiety, and feel less autonomy than those who didn’t tie their
self-worth to income, regardless of their actual economic status.
“People in this society are often focused on
pursuing money, and they don’t think there is anything bad about that,” says
Park. “But in terms of your psychological well-being, there are all kinds of
negative consequences.”
It also might affect your problem-solving
ability. Park and her colleagues randomly assigned participants to write about
their dissatisfaction with either an aspect of their financial situation—like
not having enough money to pay rent—or their academic performance, like getting
a bad test grade. Afterwards, they reported on what coping strategies they
would use in response to the situation.
Research assistants analyzed the essays and
found that participants who scored high in FCSW used more emotionally negative
words and reported more disengagement strategies—like giving up or avoiding
solutions—when writing about a financial stressor versus an academic stressor
than people scoring low in FCSW. None of the results were affected by the
actual income of the students.
People who are facing a problem should,
logically, be focused on figuring out ways to solve it, says Park. “But what we
found is that high financial contingency of self-worth somehow blocks that
response.”
Why would this be?
Park believes that when people feel their self-concept
is threatened in some way, they will become more self-protective so as not to
experience low self-esteem. So, if your self-esteem is tied to money, a
financial stressor will cause a lot more stress than it would for someone who
doesn’t feel that way. Some support for her argument comes from another part of
her experiment, where having participants high in FCSW remind themselves of
their character strengths—like their intelligence or sense of humor—seemed to
negate these avoidance effects.
Affirming Important Values
“When
people take a step back and have a broader perspective on their sense of self,
that’s often enough to take them out of the self-esteem rumination/narrow focus
they otherwise have,” says Park.
As prior research suggests, it can also be
the case that people simply want money to do something that it cannot.
“Self-esteem, like happiness, is a byproduct of meeting psychological
needs—like meaning or purpose, feeling competent, having close relationships,
or having a sense of autonomy—and basing your self-worth on financial success
actually detracts from fulfilling those needs,” says Park.
Why
community beats money
Park’s findings mirror other recent findings
from the University of San Francisco’s Matthew Monnot, who studied financial
success and well-being in China.
In his study,
Monnot notes that, as China’s economy has grown, its citizens seem to be facing
some of the same issues that Americans have faced. A growing number of
people equate individual
success with making more money and valuing money—an extrinsic reward—over
other, more intrinsic rewards, like relationships or community. To see how this
trend has affected well-being, he conducted a series of studies involving
thousands of participants from several cities in China.
In one experiment, Monnot showed that job
satisfaction did not rise in tandem with income. In fact, as with prior
studies, wealth beyond a certain point tended to make Chinese workers no more
satisfied with their jobs or their incomes, suggesting that money has only so
much power to increase our life satisfaction.
“Our findings are pretty aligned with prior research,”
says Monnot. “The correlation between income and job satisfaction is really
small, to the extent that it predicts about five percent of job satisfaction.”
To tease out why, Monnot looked at how
individual values shape the relationship between money and income satisfaction.
He asked participants to pick the five factors they thought were most important
for well-being from a list of 25 possible choices—including income—and then
measured how satisfied they were with their income as it rose. Though one might
expect people valuing income to be happier as they made more money, Monnot
found the opposite: People who picked income as an important value were
significantly less satisfied, even at higher levels.
“If income is important to you, then income
is actually less satisfying as income goes up than if income is not important
to you,” he says.
Though this seems paradoxical, Monnot says it
makes some sense, when you understand how intrinsic versus extrinsic values
affect our happiness.
“Not all goals are equal in terms of
producing well-being, productivity, job satisfaction, or life satisfaction,”
says Monnot. “If you say income is something really important to you, because
income is an extrinsic reward and not part of your intrinsic needs, if you
focus on it, it won’t make you happy.”
In other words, look inside of yourself, not
your wallet, for happiness.
Monnot was curious to see how living in
different cities in China might affect the relationship between valuing income
and well-being. After all, if the government is developing policies to increase
income in certain areas of the country, it would be good to know the impact
this is having on happiness.
Again, he had people pick out five things
they valued; but this time he separated people into different groups depending
on whether they placed high value on materialistic things, like income and status,
and placed low value on relationships and community, or vice versa. When he
compared these two groups, people who valued relationships or community versus
materialistic things had greater job satisfaction and overall life
satisfaction. This was true regardless of their city’s per-capita income.
“The big idea is that higher GDP or people
having more money is all great—especially at a societal level. You want your
economy to be more productive and to have more resources and money available,”
says Monnot. “Well, maybe that’s not necessarily in and of itself something
that’s going to produce a happier population.”
Both Monnot and Park hope that their research
might lead people to think a bit differently about the supposed benefits of
striving for more money. Though neither would deny that we need money to
survive, valuing it too highly or tying it to your self-worth is clearly a
mistake.
Monnot hopes his research might help
individuals—and business leaders and policymakers—to realize that fulfilling
psychological needs is more important to happiness than making a lot of money.
“Autonomy,
developing a skill set to be good at what you do, being affiliative with
others, having a sense of connection to your community—these are all things
that we as researchers are fairly convinced are innate, evolved human
tendencies that bring happiness,” he says.
“If you can get people to focus on fulfilling
those needs, they’ll become happier. The research is strongly in favor of
that.”
BY JILL SUTTIE |
OCTOBER 30, 2017
https://greatergood.berkeley.edu/article/item/how_does_valuing_money_affect_your_happiness?utm_source=Greater+Good+Science+Center&utm_campaign=cc55c44486-EMAIL_CAMPAIGN_2017_11_01&utm_medium=email&utm_term=0_5ae73e326e-cc55c44486-51482775
No comments:
Post a Comment