How to test your decision-making instincts
Executives
should trust their gut instincts—but only when four tests are met.
One of the most
important questions facing
leaders is when they should trust their gut instincts—an issue explored in a
dialogue between Nobel laureate Daniel Kahneman and psychologist Gary Klein
titled “Strategic decisions: When can you trust your gut?” published by McKinsey
Quarterly in March 2010. Our work on flawed decisions suggests that
leaders cannot prevent gut instinct from influencing their judgments. What they
can do is identify situations where it is likely to be biased and then
strengthen the decision process to reduce the resulting risk.
Our gut intuition
accesses our accumulated experiences in a synthesized way, so that we can form
judgments and take action without any logical, conscious consideration. Think
about how we react when we inadvertently drive across the center line in a road
or see a car start to pull out of a side turn unexpectedly. Our bodies are
jolted alert, and we turn the steering wheel well before we have had time to
think about what the appropriate reaction should be.
The brain appears to
work in a similar way when we make more leisurely decisions. In fact, the
latest findings in decision neuroscience suggest that our judgments are
initiated by the unconscious weighing of emotional tags associated with our
memories rather than by the conscious weighing of rational pros and cons: we
start to feel something—often even before we are conscious of
having thought anything. As a highly cerebral academic
colleague recently commented, “I can’t see a logical flaw in what you are
saying, but it gives me a queasy feeling in my stomach.”
Given the powerful
influence of positive and negative emotions on our unconscious, it is tempting
to argue that leaders should never trust their gut: they should make decisions
based solely on objective, logical analysis. But this advice overlooks the fact
that we can’t get away from the influence of our gut instincts. They influence
the way we frame a situation. They influence the options we choose to analyze.
They cause us to consult some people and pay less attention to others. They
encourage us to collect more data in one area but not in another. They
influence the amount of time and effort we put into decisions. In other words,
they infiltrate our decision making even when we are trying to be analytical
and rational.
This means that to
protect decisions against bias, we first need to know when we can trust our gut
feelings, confident that they are drawing on appropriate experiences and
emotions. There are four tests.
1.
The
familiarity test: Have we frequently experienced identical or similar
situations?
Familiarity is important because our subconscious works on pattern recognition. If we have plenty of appropriate memories to scan, our judgment is likely to be sound; chess masters can make good chess moves in as few as six seconds. “Appropriate” is the key word here because many disastrous decisions have been based on experiences that turned out to be misleading—for instance, the decision General Matthew Broderick, an official of the US Department of Homeland Security, made on August 29, 2005, to delay initiating the Federal response following Hurricane Katrina.
The way to judge appropriate familiarity is by examining the main uncertainties in a situation—do we have sufficient experience to make sound judgments about them? The main uncertainties facing Broderick were about whether the levees had been breached and how much danger people faced in New Orleans. Unfortunately, his previous experience with hurricanes was in cities above sea level. His learned response, of waiting for “ground truth,” proved disastrous.
Gary Klein’s premortem technique, a way of identifying why a project could fail, helps surface these uncertainties. But we can also just develop a list of uncertainties and assess whether we have sufficient experience to judge them well.
Familiarity is important because our subconscious works on pattern recognition. If we have plenty of appropriate memories to scan, our judgment is likely to be sound; chess masters can make good chess moves in as few as six seconds. “Appropriate” is the key word here because many disastrous decisions have been based on experiences that turned out to be misleading—for instance, the decision General Matthew Broderick, an official of the US Department of Homeland Security, made on August 29, 2005, to delay initiating the Federal response following Hurricane Katrina.
The way to judge appropriate familiarity is by examining the main uncertainties in a situation—do we have sufficient experience to make sound judgments about them? The main uncertainties facing Broderick were about whether the levees had been breached and how much danger people faced in New Orleans. Unfortunately, his previous experience with hurricanes was in cities above sea level. His learned response, of waiting for “ground truth,” proved disastrous.
Gary Klein’s premortem technique, a way of identifying why a project could fail, helps surface these uncertainties. But we can also just develop a list of uncertainties and assess whether we have sufficient experience to judge them well.
2.
The
feedback test: Did we get reliable feedback in past situations?
Previous experience is useful to us only if we learned the right lessons. At the time we make a decision, our brains tag it with a positive emotion—recording it as a good judgment. Hence, without reliable feedback, our emotional tags can tell us that our past judgments were good, even though an objective assessment would record them as bad. For example, if we change jobs before the impact of a judgment is clear or if we have people filtering the information we receive and protecting us from bad news, we may not get the feedback we need. It is for this reason that “yes men” around leaders are so pernicious: they often eliminate the feedback process so important to the development of appropriate emotional tags.
Previous experience is useful to us only if we learned the right lessons. At the time we make a decision, our brains tag it with a positive emotion—recording it as a good judgment. Hence, without reliable feedback, our emotional tags can tell us that our past judgments were good, even though an objective assessment would record them as bad. For example, if we change jobs before the impact of a judgment is clear or if we have people filtering the information we receive and protecting us from bad news, we may not get the feedback we need. It is for this reason that “yes men” around leaders are so pernicious: they often eliminate the feedback process so important to the development of appropriate emotional tags.
3.
The
measured-emotions test: Are the emotions we have experienced in similar or
related situations measured?
All memories come with emotional tags, but some are more highly charged than others. If a situation brings to mind highly charged emotions, these can unbalance our judgment. Knowing from personal experience that dogs can bite is different from having a traumatic childhood experience with dogs. The first will help you interact with dogs. The second can make you afraid of even the friendliest dog.
A board chairman, for example, had personally lost a significant amount of money with a previous company when doing business in Russia. This traumatic experience made him wary of a proposal for a major Russian expansion in his new company. But he also realized that the experience could be biasing his judgment. He felt obliged to share his concerns but then asked the rest of the board to make the final decision.
All memories come with emotional tags, but some are more highly charged than others. If a situation brings to mind highly charged emotions, these can unbalance our judgment. Knowing from personal experience that dogs can bite is different from having a traumatic childhood experience with dogs. The first will help you interact with dogs. The second can make you afraid of even the friendliest dog.
A board chairman, for example, had personally lost a significant amount of money with a previous company when doing business in Russia. This traumatic experience made him wary of a proposal for a major Russian expansion in his new company. But he also realized that the experience could be biasing his judgment. He felt obliged to share his concerns but then asked the rest of the board to make the final decision.
4.
The
independence test: Are we likely to be influenced by any inappropriate personal
interests or attachments?
If we are trying to decide between two office locations for an organization, one of which is much more personally convenient, we should be cautious. Our subconscious will have more positive emotional tags for the more convenient location. It is for this reason that it is standard practice to ask board members with personal interests in a particular decision to leave the meeting or to refrain from voting. Also for this reason, we enjoy the quip “turkeys will not vote for Christmas.”
A similar logic applies to personal attachments. When auditors, for example, were asked to demonstrate to a Harvard professor that their professional training enabled them to be objective in arriving at an audit opinion, regardless of the nature of the relationship they had with a company, they demonstrated the opposite.
If we are trying to decide between two office locations for an organization, one of which is much more personally convenient, we should be cautious. Our subconscious will have more positive emotional tags for the more convenient location. It is for this reason that it is standard practice to ask board members with personal interests in a particular decision to leave the meeting or to refrain from voting. Also for this reason, we enjoy the quip “turkeys will not vote for Christmas.”
A similar logic applies to personal attachments. When auditors, for example, were asked to demonstrate to a Harvard professor that their professional training enabled them to be objective in arriving at an audit opinion, regardless of the nature of the relationship they had with a company, they demonstrated the opposite.
If a situation fails
even one of these four tests, we need to strengthen the decision process to
reduce the risk of a bad outcome. There are usually three ways of doing
this—stronger governance, additional experience and data, or more dialogue and
challenge. Often, strong governance, in the form of a boss who can overrule a
judgment, is the best safeguard. But a strong governance process can be hard to
set up and expensive to maintain (think of the US Senate or a typical corporate
board). So it is normally cheaper to look for safeguards based on experience
and data or on dialogue and challenge.
In the 1990s, for
example, Jack Welch knew he would face some tough decisions about how to
exploit the Internet, so he chose experience as a solution to the biases he
might have. He hired a personal Internet mentor who was more than 25 years his
junior and encouraged his top managers to do the same. Warren Buffett
recommends extra challenge as a solution to biases that arise during
acquisitions. Whenever a company is paying part of the price with shares, he
proposes using an “adviser against the deal,” who would be compensated well
only if it did not go through.
There are no universal safeguards.
Premortems help surface uncertainties, but they do not protect against
self-interest. Additional data can challenge assumptions but will not help a
decision maker who is influenced by a strong emotional experience. If we are to
make better decisions, we need to be thoughtful both about why our gut
instincts might let us down and what the best safeguard is in each situation.
We should never ignore our gut. But we should know when to rely on it and when
to safeguard against it.
By Andrew Campbell and Jo Whitehead
http://www.mckinsey.com/business-functions/strategy-and-corporate-finance/our-insights/how-to-test-your-decision-making-instincts?cid=other-eml-cls-mkq-mck-oth-1609
No comments:
Post a Comment