The problem with first impressions
We are born to judge others by how they look: our
brains come hardwired with a specific face-processing area, and even shortly
after birth, babies would rather look at a human face than anything else. Within
their first year, they become more discerning, and are more likely to crawl
towards friendly looking faces than those who look a bit shifty. By the time we
reach adulthood, we are snap-judgement specialists, jumping to conclusions
about a person's character and status after seeing their face for just a tenth
of a second. And we shun considered assessments of others in favour of simple
shortcuts – for example, we judge a baby-faced individual as more trustworthy,
and associate a chiselled jaw with dominance.
Unfair, it may be, but it makes good evolutionary
sense. Ours is an ultra-social species, so being able to quickly assess whether
someone is friend or foe and whether they have the power to help or hurt us is
important survival information. But there is a problem. As psychologist
Alexander Todorov of Princeton University points out, more often than not, our
first impressions are wrong. He suggests that poor feedback and the fact that
we meet more strangers than our prehistoric ancestors would have, both play a
part.
Another problem is that we don’t stick to
stereotyping faces one at a time. We are just as quick to categorise groups of
people and then discriminate against them as a result. Research by Susan Fiske,
also of Princeton, and her colleagues has shown that group stereotypes, too,
are based on levels of trustworthiness and status. The researchers plotted
these categories on a two-by-two grid, each quarter of which is associated with
a particular emotion: pity, disgust, pride or envy. This, they found, informs
our behaviour towards people in the group.
Their findings don’t paint us in a great light. We
tend to dehumanise groups we judge to be lacking in warmth, and react violently
to those with high status. “Historically, many genocides have been directed
towards groups that fall into the envy quadrant,” says Fiske. Even our
relatively positive reactions have downsides: we may pity those of low status,
but react by patronising them, and the pride we feel towards our own group can
spill over into nepotism.
If you think you are above this kind of thing, think
again. Even if you consciously reject stereotypes, the culture you live in does
not, and experiments suggest that you are likely to share its biases.
The best way to escape this evolutionary trap is to
really get to know people from outside your echo chamber. Working together is
ideal because relying on someone forces you to look beyond simplistic first
impressions. And don’t trust social stereotypes – even your own national
stereotype. The evidence suggests that we are not even accurate when it comes
to judging ourselves.
New Scientist
2017, Tribune Content Agency
2017, Tribune Content Agency
1 comment:
We are not accurate even in judging ourselves. summarizes all !
Post a Comment