The
Unconscious Executive
How unconscious processes improve decision-making. Conscious
deliberation, it turns out, does not always lead to the best outcomes.
What strategy do you
use when making complex decisions? Do you burn the midnight oil to carefully
weigh the pros and cons of each alternative? Or do you take your mind off the
decision and follow the age-old wisdom of "sleeping on it"?
Both decision-making
methods have strengths and weaknesses, says Harvard Business School
postdoctoral fellow Maarten Bos. Our conscious mind is pretty good at following
rules, but our unconscious mind—our ability to "think without
attention"—can handle a larger amount of information. Studying the
unconscious mind offers exciting new avenues for research, including
creativity, decision making, and sleep.
Research by Bos and
his colleagues suggests that unconscious thought supports the kind of mental
organization needed for making complex decisions. In addition, unconscious
thought might be more dependable than conscious thought when we are low on
energy. Preliminary research also indicates that using odor or sound cues
during sleep might activate our unconscious mind and improve creativity and
innovation.
Bos, a social
psychologist, conducts his research at HBS, in close collaboration with Harvard
Medical School. In addition to researching sleep, creativity, and decision
making, he studies influence, persuasion, voice and facial categorization, and
communication.
Martha
Lagace: What do you mean by unconscious thought?
Maarten Bos: We
define unconscious thought as a goal-dependent, deliberative process in the
absence of conscious attention. Most people attribute a lot of their actions to
a conscious process, but there are scores of processes that operate
unconsciously. Getting dressed in the morning, for example, is largely an
unconscious process. So is driving to work—many people get to work without
entirely remembering how their drive was and what they saw on the road.
Lots of processes are
automated and therefore very efficient. Our research shows thinking and
deciding can also often be left successfully to the unconscious mind.
Here is an example of
unconscious thought. Imagine you are listening to a song and can't remember the
name of the artist. You try to think hard, but are still unable to come up with
it. So you tell yourself, "I'll stop thinking about it, and it will come
to me in a minute." This is fascinating. In fact, there is an automatic
process that continues to work on your question in the back of your mind. We
call that process "unconscious thought."
Unconscious thought
can do more than just help you remember facts. It actually has the power to
fuel the creative process. Have you ever found yourself struggling with the
wording while writing a paper, but after taking time away from it, you're able
to quickly find the right words? This is your unconscious mind at work.
While our conscious
mind is focused on other matters, our unconscious mind can process the relevant
information we need to make important decisions.
Q: As you
highlight in an article about one of your research studies, "sleeping on
it" is good folk wisdom for making decisions. Yet people often feel the
need to carefully ponder all options and thus choose rationally. What does your
research suggest about trying to navigate these two seemingly contradictory
views?
A: Yes, there
seems to be a discrepancy. People think, on the one hand, they should carefully
consider all options before making a decision, but on the other hand, we say
that we should 'sleep on our decisions.' My colleagues and I think both are
true, and that careful consideration of all the options can be done by our
unconscious mind.
Both conscious and
unconscious thought have strengths and weaknesses. There are decisions where we
believe conscious thought outperforms unconscious thought. For
example, when a decision requires application of very strict, mathematical
rules, we hypothesize that conscious thought is beneficial. But when it comes
to integrating a large amount of information, we
think unconscious thought, which gives rougher estimates, is more beneficial
(see our article in Science for more information).
Q: One way you
and colleagues have tried to sort out the mechanisms of conscious or
unconscious thought yielding better results in particular decisions has been by
running experiments that alter blood glucose levels. Could you tell us more
about that?
A: We ran experiments where we manipulated the amount of sugar people ingested
in a three-hour time period. We showed that while giving people a drink with
sugar helps conscious thought—not a strange finding, considering our brain is
fueled by glucose—surprisingly, unconscious thought performs better when people
receive a drink without sugar in it. It seems that unconscious thought is not as
dependent on glucose as conscious thought.
Other research
conducted by [Mareike] Wieth and [Rose] Zacks has shown people are very good at
making intuitive decisions at times when they are not at their best according
to their circadian clock. In this research participants showed greater insight and creative problem-solving
performance during nonoptimal times of day compared to optimal times of day. It
seems that conscious thought sometimes gets in the way of making good
decisions.
Q: You and your
colleagues also study sleep and creative performance, as you write in an
article in the Journal of Sleep Research. You take the interesting tack of
studying odor. Why?
A: We used odor
as a cue to reactivate thoughts about a task. This cue has the potential to
increase information processing during sleep—a finding with powerful practical
applications.
First, the cue is
paired with a task, then the cue is repeated during sleep. To give an example:
Imagine that you are reading materials for an important meeting right before
you go to sleep. While you are reading, a cue is produced—either a smell is
dispersed in the room or a distinct sound is played. Then during the night,
that same cue is produced. The cue reactivates the reading you did before going
to sleep and improves your memory of the information. This strategy could
potentially act as a study aid for students and professionals alike.
So far, though, we
have only found results for creativity. People were given instructions for a
creativity task, paired with a cue, before they went to sleep. Those
participants who were given the same cue at night were more creative the next
morning.
Q: What are you
working on next?
A: These are very
exciting times. There is so much to do! One question we are investigating is
which sleep phases are most sensitive to these conditioned cues I just
mentioned. Replaying a cue all night might make the cue less effective.
(Imagine putting on cologne in the morning. After about 20 seconds you don't
smell it anymore because you've gotten used to it.) We don't know if this
replaying affects the effectiveness of the cue. We also don't know if people
get used to sounds or music in the same way. If we find out in which sleep
phase these cues are most effective, then we may learn more about memory
processing during sleep. We also need to do more research to find out if sleep
quality is adversely affected by the cues, but no results so far indicate that
sleep is disrupted.
Another route we're
investigating is whether this cue-activated boost works for decision making
like it did for creativity. Creativity can be a very divergent process, while
decision making is a more convergent process. We don't know for sure if sleep
works the same, or as beneficially, for both of these processes.
I'm also working on
various other lines of research that are less related to sleep. But whether
it's sleep research (with Harvard Medical School's Robert Stickgold and Harvard
Kennedy School's Todd Rogers, HBS PhDOB'08), influence and persuasion research
(with my main collaborator at HBS, professor Amy Cuddy), voice analysis research
(with Amy Cuddy and Cornell's Tanzeem Choudhury and her student Mashfiqui
Rabbi), decision-making research (with Radboud University's Ap Dijksterhuis and
Kellogg's Loran Nordgren), creativity research (with Harvard Psychology's
Adrian Ward and Catalyst's Anna Beninger), or body posture research (with Amy
Cuddy and MIT's Ehsan Hoque), almost all the research I do is about the
unconscious and cognitive performance, and I really think it's fascinating
stuff.
I'll leave you with
this. We sleep about a quarter to a third of our lives. Imagine how powerful it
would be if we could make that time more useful? If the reactivation works the
way we think it does—and the way our data show it works—then the applications
are endless. We could tap into the vast potential of the unconscious, while
we're comfortably sleeping in our beds.
by Martha Lagace
http://hbswk.hbs.edu/item/6872.html
No comments:
Post a Comment